Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
M. Ruiz, E. Drake, D. Marcottee, A. Glass, W. Gorp (2000)
Trying to beat the system: misuse of the internet to assist in avoiding the detection of neuropsychological symptom dissimulationArchives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 15
M. Haines, M. Norris (1995)
Detecting the malingering of cognitive deficits: An updateNeuropsychology Review, 5
E. Vakil, H. Blachstein (1994)
A supplementary measure in the Rey AVLT for assessing incidental learning of temporal order.Journal of clinical psychology, 50 2
J. Gillis, R. Rogers, R. Bagby (1991)
Validity of the M Test: simulation-design and natural-group approaches.Journal of personality assessment, 57 1
N. Klimczak, P. Donovick, R. Burright (1997)
The malingering of multiple sclerosis and mild traumatic brain injury.Brain injury, 11 5
M. Lezak (1995)
Neuropsychological assessment, 3rd ed.
L. Bernard, W. Houston, Lisa Natoli (1993)
Malingering on neuropsychological memory tests: potential objective indicators.Journal of clinical psychology, 49 1
J. Tan, D. Slick, E. Strauss, D. Hultsch (2002)
How'd They Do It? Malingering Strategies on Symptom Validity TestsThe Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16
M. Greiffenstein, T. Gola, W. Baker (1995)
MMPI-2 validity scales versus domain specific measures in detection of factitious traumatic brain injuryClinical Neuropsychologist, 9
S. Essig, W. Mittenberg, R. Petersen, S. Strauman, J. Cooper (1999)
Practices in forensic neuropsychology: perspectives of neuropsychologists and trial attorneys.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 16 3
D. Sherman, K. Boone, P. Lu, J. Razani (2002)
Re-Examination of a Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test/Rey Complex Figure Discriminant Function to Detect Suspect EffortThe Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16
E. Vakil, R. Jaffe, Shira Eluze, Z. Groswasser, S. Aberbuch (1996)
Word Recall versus Reading Speed: Evidence of Preserved Priming in Head-Injured PatientsBrain and Cognition, 31
M. Mitrushina, K. Boone, L. D’Elia (1999)
Handbook of Normative Data for Neuropsychological Assessment
L. Bernard (1990)
Prospects for faking believable memory deficits on neuropsychological tests and the use of incentives in simulation research.Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 12 5
D. Slick, E. Sherman, G. Iverson (1999)
Diagnostic criteria for malingered neurocognitive dysfunction: proposed standards for clinical practice and research.The Clinical neuropsychologist, 13 4
David Shum, Elizabeth Jamieson, Mark Bahr, G. Wallace (1999)
Implicit and explicit memory in children with traumatic brain injury.Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 21 2
M. Ruiz, E. Drake, A. Glass, David Marcotte, W. Gorp (2002)
Trying to beat the system: Misuse of the Internet to assist in avoiding the detection of psychological symptom dissimulationProfessional Psychology: Research and Practice, 33
P. Rawling, N. Brooks (1990)
Simulation Index: A method for detecting factitious errors on the WAIS--R and WMS.Neuropsychology (journal), 4
P. Graf, L. Squire, G. Mandler (1984)
The information that amnesic patients do not forget.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 10 1
J. Meyers, M. Volbrecht (2003)
A validation of multiple malingering detection methods in a large clinical sample.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 18 3
L. Bernard (1991)
The detection of faked deficits on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: the effect of serial position.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 6 1-2
G. Iverson, M. Franzen (1994)
The Recognition Memory Test, Digit Span, and Knox Cube Test as Markers of Malingered Memory ImpairmentAssessment, 1
M. Greiffenstein, W. Baker, T. Gola (1994)
Validation of malingered amnesia measures with a large clinical samplePsychological Assessment, 6
L. Hasher, R. Zacks (1979)
Automatic and effortful processes in memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108
M. Chouinard, I. Rouleau (1997)
The 48-Pictures Test: A two-alternative forced-choice recognition test for the detection of malingeringJournal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3
Hasker Davis, John King, K. Klebe, George Bajszár, M. Bloodworth, Sarah Wallick (1997)
The detection of simulated malingering using a computerized priming test.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 12 2
M. Greiffenstein, W. Baker, T. Gola (1996)
Comparison of multiple scoring methods for Rey's malingered amnesia measures.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 11 4
K. Boone, P. Lu, C. Back, C. King, Alison Lee, L. Philpott, E. Shamieh, K. Warner-Chacon (2002)
Sensitivity and specificity of the Rey Dot Counting Test in patients with suspect effort and various clinical samples.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 17 7
T. Inman, D. Berry (2002)
Cross-validation of indicators of malingering: a comparison of nine neuropsychological tests, four tests of malingering, and behavioral observations.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 17 1
J. Meyers, Amber Morrison, Joseph Miller (2001)
How Low Is Too Low, Revisited: Sentence Repetition and AVLT-Recognition in the Detection of MalingeringApplied Neuropsychology, 8
J. King, J. Gfeller, H. Davis (1998)
Detecting simulated memory impairment with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test: implications of base rates and study generalizability.Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 20 5
E. Vakil, H. Blachstein, D. Hoofien (1991)
Automatic temporal order judgment: the effect of intentionality of retrieval on closed-head-injured patients.Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 13 2
L. Binder, Michael Villanueva, D. Howieson, Ray Moore (1993)
The Rey AVLT recognition memory task measures motivational impairment after mild head trauma.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 8 2
K. Flowers, M. Sheridan, H. Shadbolt (1996)
Simulation of amnesia by normals on Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning TestJournal of Neurolinguistics, 9
M. Haines, M. Norris (2001)
Comparing Student and Patient Simulated Malingerers Performance on Standard Neuropsychological Measures to Detect Feigned Cognitive DeficitsThe Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15
E. Wiggins, J. Brandt (1988)
The detection of simulated amnesiaLaw and Human Behavior, 12
K. Nies, J. Sweet (1994)
Neuropsychological assessment and malingering: a critical review of past and present strategies.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 9 6
L. Binder, M. Kelly, Michael Villanueva, M. Winslow (2003)
Motivation and Neuropsychological Test Performance Following Mild Head InjuryJournal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25
J. Suhr (2002)
Malingering, coaching, and the serial position effect.Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 17 1
Julie Suhr, Daniel Tranel, J. Wefel, J. Barrash (1997)
Memory performance after head injury: contributions of malingering, litigation status, psychological factors, and medication use.Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 19 4
AbstractSixty-one noncredible patients (as documented by psychometric and behavioral criteria) scored significantly below 25 controls and 88 credible clinic patients with no motive to feign on most RAVLT scores, including added indices of implicit and “automatic” memory. A combination of true recognition (i.e., recognition minus false positives) + implicit memory score (i.e., the number of word stems completed with RAVLT items) + “automatic” memory score (i.e., the number of correct temporal order judgments) ≤22 was associated with 75.7% sensitivity with specificity at 91.5%. However, sensitivity was nearly as high when scores available from the standard RAVLT administration alone (i.e., no word stem or temporal order trials) were combined. Specifically, a cut-off of ≤12 for true recognition (recognition minus false positives) + primacy recognition (i.e., number of words recognized from the first third of the test) was associated with 73.8% sensitivity at 90% specificity. These results indicate that combined indices of recognition memory from the RAVLT are effective in identifying noncredible memory performance in “real world” samples and are modestly superior to the 67.2% sensitivity obtained with the standard recognition score.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology – Oxford University Press
Published: May 1, 2005
Keywords: RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test Suspect effort Malingering
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.