Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
C. Agrillo, M. Dadda, A. Bisazza (2006)
Quantity discrimination in female mosquitofishAnimal Cognition, 10
D. Biro, T. Matsuzawa (2001)
Use of numerical symbols by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Cardinals, ordinals, and the introduction of zeroAnimal Cognition, 4
(2015)
RStudio: integrated development for R
I. Pepperberg (2010)
Evidence for Conceptual Quantitative Abilities in the African Grey Parrot: Labeling of Cardinal SetsEthology, 75
R. Rugani, G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin (2013)
Numerical Abstraction in Young Domestic Chicks (Gallus gallus)PLoS ONE, 8
R. Rugani, M. Lunghi, E. Giorgio, L. Regolin, B. Barba, G. Vallortigara, F. Simion (2017)
A mental number line in human newbornsbioRxiv
A. Olthof, C. Iden, W. Roberts (1997)
Judgments of ordinality and summation of number symbols by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus).Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 23
I. Pepperberg (2000)
The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots
R. Rugani, K. McCrink, M. Hevia, G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin (2016)
Ratio abstraction over discrete magnitudes by newly hatched domestic chicks (Gallus gallus)Scientific Reports, 6
C. Nicol (2015)
The Behavioural Biology of Chickens
Laura Fontanari, R. Rugani, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2011)
Object individuation in 3-day-old chicks: use of property and spatiotemporal information.Developmental science, 14 5
A. Nieder (2020)
The Adaptive Value of Numerical Competence.Trends in ecology & evolution, 35 7
(2017)
pwr: Basic functions for power analysis
L. Xia, J. Emmerton, M. Siemann, J. Delius (2001)
Pigeons (Columba livia) learn to link numerosities with symbols.Journal of comparative psychology, 115 1
M. Cailotto, G. Vallortigara, M. Zanforlin (1989)
Sex differences in the response to social stimuli in young chicksEthology Ecology & Evolution, 1
J. Pinheiro, D. Bates, R-core (1998)
LINEAR AND NONLINEAR MIXED-EFFECTS MODELS
R. Rugani, G. Vallortigara, K. Priftis, L. Regolin (2015)
Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number lineScience, 347
L. Ortman, Ter Pelkwyk (1953)
Visual patterns in the recognition of individuals among chickensThe Condor, 55
I. Pepperberg (1994)
Numerical competence in an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus).Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108
G. Vallortigara, R. Andrew (1991)
Lateralization of response by chicks to change in a model partnerAnimal Behaviour, 41
Sarah Benson-Amram, V. Heinen, S. Dryer, K. Holekamp (2011)
Numerical assessment and individual call discrimination by wild spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocutaAnimal Behaviour, 82
H. Pulliam (1973)
On the advantages of flocking.Journal of theoretical biology, 38 2
(2020)
tidyr: tidy messy data
Sarie Belle, Clara Scarry (2015)
Individual participation in intergroup contests is mediated by numerical assessment strategies in black howler and tufted capuchin monkeysPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370
M. Bortot, C. Agrillo, A. Avarguès-Weber, A. Bisazza, M. Petrazzini, M. Giurfa (2019)
Honeybees use absolute rather than relative numerosity in number discriminationBiology Letters, 15
Self-Concept Variables (2016)
Sex Differences in
H. Wickham (2011)
The Split-Apply-Combine Strategy for Data AnalysisJournal of Statistical Software, 40
Bezawork Bogale, M. Aoyama, S. Sugita (2014)
Spontaneous discrimination of food quantities in the jungle crow, Corvus macrorhynchosAnimal Behaviour, 94
T. Lucon‐Xiccato, M. Petrazzini, C. Agrillo, A. Bisazza (2015)
Guppies discriminate between two quantities of food items but prioritize item size over total amountAnimal Behaviour, 107
R. Rugani, G. Vallortigara, B. Vallini, L. Regolin (2011)
Asymmetrical number-space mapping in the avian brainNeurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95
(1935)
Social behavior of birds
R. Rugani, M. Loconsole, F. Simion, L. Regolin (2020)
Individually distinctive features facilitate numerical discrimination of sets of objects in domestic chicksScientific Reports, 10
R. Rugani, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2011)
Summation of Large Numerousness by Newborn ChicksFrontiers in Psychology, 2
B. Diekamp, L. Regolin, O. Güntürkün, G. Vallortigara (2005)
A left-sided visuospatial bias in birdsCurrent Biology, 15
C. Gallistel (1989)
Animal cognition: the representation of space, time and number.Annual review of psychology, 40
Kira Cassidy, D. MacNulty, D. Stahler, Douglas Smith, David Mech (2015)
Group composition effects on aggressive interpack interactions of gray wolves in Yellowstone National ParkBehavioral Ecology, 26
R. Rugani, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2010)
Imprinted numbers: newborn chicks' sensitivity to number vs. continuous extent of objects they have been reared with.Developmental science, 13 5
M. Hauser, S. Carey, Lilan Hauser (2000)
Spontaneous number representation in semi–free–ranging rhesus monkeysProceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 267
(2017)
An animal’s sense of number
R. Rugani, D. Kelly, Izabela Szelest, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2010)
Is it only humans that count from left to right?Biology Letters, 6
R. Gottier (1968)
The dominance-submission hierarchy in the social behavior of the domestic chicken.The Journal of genetic psychology, 112 2d Half
G. Vallortigara, M. Cailotto, M. Zanforlin (1990)
Sex differences in social reinstatement motivation of the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) revealed by runway tests with social and nonsocial reinforcement.Journal of comparative psychology, 104 4
R. Rugani, M. Loconsole, L. Regolin (2017)
A strategy to improve arithmetical performance in four day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus)Scientific Reports, 7
L. Regolin (2006)
The Case of the Line-Bisection: When Both Humans and Chickens Wander LeftCortex, 42
Tsang-I Yang, C. Chiao (2016)
Number sense and state-dependent valuation in cuttlefishProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283
R. Bonanni, E. Natoli, S. Cafazzo, P. Valsecchi (2010)
Free-ranging dogs assess the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflictsAnimal Cognition, 14
L. Workman, R. Andrew (1989)
Simultaneous changes in behaviour and in lateralization during the development of male and female domestic chicksAnimal Behaviour, 38
R. Rugani (2018)
Towards numerical cognition's origin: insights from day-old domestic chicksPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373
I. Pepperberg (1988)
Comprehension of "absence" by an African Grey parrot: Learning with respect to questions of same/different.Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 50 3
M. Gibbs, C. Gibbs, A. Csillag, T. Matsushima, L. Rogers, J. Rostas (2008)
Brain mechanisms, cognition and behaviour in birdsBrain Research Bulletin, 76
R. Rugani, O. Salva, L. Regolin (2014)
Lateralized mechanisms for encoding of object. Behavioral evidence from an animal model: the domestic chick (Gallus gallus)Frontiers in Psychology, 5
R. Rugani, G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin (2014)
The use of proportion by young domestic chicks (Gallus gallus)Animal Cognition, 18
Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations
A. Smirnova, O. Lazareva, Z. Zorina (2000)
Use of number by crows: investigation by matching and oddity learning.Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 73 2
A. Nieder (2019)
A Brain for Numbers: The Biology of the Number Instinct
J. Bolhuis (1991)
MECHANISMS OF AVIAN IMPRINTING: A REVIEWBiological Reviews, 66
Davide Potrich, V. Sovrano, G. Stancher, G. Vallortigara (2015)
Quantity discrimination by zebrafish (Danio rerio).Journal of comparative psychology, 129 4
R. Rugani, A. Cavazzana, G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin (2013)
One, two, three, four, or is there something more? Numerical discrimination in day-old domestic chicksAnimal Cognition, 16
E. Versace, Michelle Spierings, M. Caffini, C. Cate, G. Vallortigara (2017)
Spontaneous generalization of abstract multimodal patterns in young domestic chicksAnimal Cognition, 20
R. Rodríguez, bullet Briceñ, Eduardo bullet, O-Aguilar Briceñ, Gerlinde bullet, Höbel (2014)
Nephila clavipes spiders (Araneae: Nephilidae) keep track of captured prey counts: testing for a sense of numerosity in an orb-weaverAnimal Cognition, 18
B. Corte, V. Navarro, E. Wasserman (2017)
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeonsCurrent Biology, 27
G. Vallortigara (1992)
Right hemisphere advantage for social recognition in the chickNeuropsychologia, 30
Bezawork Bogale, N. Kamata, Katano Mioko, S. Sugita (2011)
Quantity discrimination in jungle crows, Corvus macrorhynchosAnimal Behaviour, 82
D. Bates, M. Machler, B. Bolker, Steven Walker (2014)
Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4Journal of Statistical Software, 67
T. Matsushima, E. Izawa, N. Aoki, S. Yanagihara (2003)
The Mind Through Chick Eyes : Memory, Cognition and Anticipation, 20
(2015)
Learning statistics with R: A tutorial for psychology
A. Gazzola, G. Vallortigara, D. Pellitteri‐Rosa (2018)
Continuous and discrete quantity discrimination in tortoisesbioRxiv
I. Pepperberg, J. Gordon (2005)
Number comprehension by a grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), including a zero-like concept.Journal of comparative psychology, 119 2
M. Hager, G. Helfman (1991)
Safety in numbers: shoal size choice by minnows under predatory threatBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29
(2017)
Birdbrains’ should not be ignored in studying the evolution of gBehavioral and Brain Sciences, 40
Simon Hunt, J. Low, K. Burns (2008)
Adaptive numerical competency in a food-hoarding songbirdProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275
G. McBride, F. Foenander (1962)
Territorial Behaviour in Flocks of Domestic FowlsNature, 194
Virgilio Gómez-Rubio (2017)
ggplot2 - Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis (2nd Edition)Journal of Statistical Software, 77
(1922)
Beiträge zur sozialpsychologie des haushuhns [Observation on the social psychology of domestic fowls
S. Queiroz, V. Cromberg (2006)
Aggressive behavior in the genus Gallus spBrazilian Journal of Poultry Science, 8
L. Regolin, R. Rugani, P. Pagni, G. Vallortigara (2005)
Delayed search for social and nonsocial goals by young domestic chicks, Gallus gallus domesticusAnimal Behaviour, 70
R. Rugani (2016)
Number-space associations without language. Behavioral evidence from an animal model: the domestic chick (Gallus gallus), 51
Helen Ditz, A. Nieder (2016)
Numerosity representations in crows obey the Weber–Fechner lawProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 283
J. Daisley, G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin (2010)
Logic in an asymmetrical (social) brain: Transitive inference in the young domestic chickSocial Neuroscience, 5
J. Faraway, G. Marsaglia, John Marsaglia, A. Baddeley (2015)
Classical Goodness-of-Fit Tests for Univariate Distributions
Dustin Merritt, R. Rugani, E. Brannon (2009)
Empty sets as part of the numerical continuum: conceptual precursors to the zero concept in rhesus monkeys.Journal of experimental psychology. General, 138 2
Dustin Merritt, Daniel Casasanto, E. Brannon (2010)
Do monkeys think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys and humansCognition, 117
R. Rugani, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2007)
Rudimental numerical competence in 5-day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus): identification of ordinal position.Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes, 33 1
Marion Mehlis, T. Thünken, T. Bakker, Joachim Frommen (2015)
Quantification acuity in spontaneous shoaling decisions of three-spined sticklebacksAnimal Cognition, 18
(2020)
dplyr: a grammar of data manipulation
J. Bolhuis, G. Horn (1992)
Generalization of learned preferences in filial imprintingAnimal Behaviour, 44
O. Güntürkün, T. Bugnyar (2016)
Cognition without CortexTrends in Cognitive Sciences, 20
H. Wickham (2007)
Reshaping Data with the reshape PackageJournal of Statistical Software, 21
R. Rugani, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2008)
Discrimination of small numerosities in young chicks.Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes, 34 3
G. Vallortigara (1992)
Affiliation and aggression as related to gender in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus).Journal of comparative psychology, 106 1
I. Pepperberg, Michael Brezinsky (1991)
Acquisition of a relative class concept by an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus): discriminations based on relative size.Journal of comparative psychology, 105 3
(2020)
tidyr: tidymessy data. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=tidyr
(2018)
Filial imprintingEncyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior
R. Rugani, G. Vallortigara, L. Regolin (2016)
Mapping number to space in the two hemispheres of the avian brainNeurobiology of Learning and Memory, 133
Marco Sandrini, E. Rusconi (2009)
A brain for numbersCortex, 45
B. McCabe (2019)
Visual Imprinting in Birds: Behavior, Models, and Neural MechanismsFrontiers in Physiology, 10
M. Bortot, G. Stancher, G. Vallortigara (2019)
Transfer from Number to Size Reveals Abstract Coding of Magnitude in HoneybeesiScience, 23
Judith Burkart, Michèle Schubiger, C. Schaik (2017)
Theory of mind: A foundational component of human general intelligenceBehavioral and Brain Sciences, 40
S. Boysen, G. Berntson (1989)
Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes).Journal of comparative psychology, 103 1
G. Roberts (1996)
Why individual vigilance declines as group size increasesAnimal Behaviour, 51
S. Yamaguchi, N. Aoki, Takaaki Kitajima, Eiji Iikubo, S. Katagiri, T. Matsushima, Koichi Homma (2012)
Thyroid hormone determines the start of the sensitive period of imprinting and primes later learningNature Communications, 3
K. McComb, C. Packer, A. Pusey (1994)
Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leoAnimal Behaviour, 47
G. McBride, I. Parer, F. Foenander (1969)
The Social Organization and Behaviour of the Feral Domestic FowlAnimal Behaviour Monographs, 2
G. Vallortigara (2015)
Chapter 2 - Foundations of Number and Space Representations in Non-Human Species, 1
Scarlett Howard, A. Avarguès-Weber, Jair Garcia, A. Greentree, A. Dyer (2018)
Numerical ordering of zero in honey beesScience, 360
R. Rugani, Laura Fontanari, E. Simoni, L. Regolin, G. Vallortigara (2009)
Arithmetic in newborn chicksProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276
M. Wilson, Sonya Kahlenberg, Michael Wells, R. Wrangham (2012)
Ecological and social factors affect the occurrence and outcomes of intergroup encounters in chimpanzeesAnimal Behaviour, 83
R. Rugani, G. Vallortigara, K. Priftis, L. Regolin (2020)
Numerical magnitude, rather than individual bias, explains spatial numerical association in newborn chickseLife, 9
Vincent Walsh (2003)
A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantityTrends in Cognitive Sciences, 7
E. Versace, G. Vallortigara (2015)
Origins of Knowledge: Insights from Precocial SpeciesFrontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9
When facing two sets of imprinting objects of different numerousness, domestic chicks prefer to approach the larger one. Given that choice for familiar and novel stimuli in imprinting situations is known to be affected by the sex of the animals, we investigated how male and female domestic chicks divide the time spent in the proximity of a familiar versus an unfamiliar number of objects, and how animals interact (by pecking) with these objects. We confirmed that chicks discriminate among the different numerousnesses, but we also showed that females and males behave differently, depending on the degree of familiarity of the objects. When objects in the testing sets were all familiar, females equally explored both sets and pecked at all objects individually. Males instead selectively approached the familiar numerousness and pecked more at it. When both testing sets comprised familiar as well as novel objects, both males and females approached the larger numerousness of familiar objects. However, chicks directed all their pecks toward the novel object within the set. Differences in the behavior of males and females can be accounted for in terms of sex difference in the motivation to reinstate social contact with the familiar objects and to explore novel ones, likely associated with the ecology and the social structure of the species before domestication. . . . Keywords Filial imprinting Domestic chicks Numerical discrimination Sex differences Introduction (Nieder, 2020). While foraging, animals from various taxa show a spontaneous preference for more food items (Bogale, The investigation of numerical cognition in animals has been Aoyama, & Sugita, 2014; Gazzola, Vallortigara, & Pellitteri- challenging. Scientists have been taking advantage of sponta- Rosa, 2018; Hauser, Carey, & Hauser, 2000; Hunt, Low, & neous choice tasks (where the animals are expected to choose Burns, 2008; Lucon-Xiccato et al., 2015; Rodríguez et al., the preferred or the most advantageous option) as well as 2015;Rugani etal., 2013a, b; Yang & Chiao, 2016). While operant conditioning tasks (Nieder, 2019). Spontaneous pref- defending their territory, animals assess the strength of the erence allows for the investigation of relative numerosity opponents by estimating their number prior to engaging in judgments (“more than” or “less than”). Using such proce- defensive displays (Benson-Amram et al., 2011; Bonanni dures, the ability to discriminate between different numerous- et al., 2011; Cassidy et al., 2015; McComb, Packer & Pusey, nesses has been described in several ecological contexts 1994; Van Belle & Scarry, 2015; Wilson et al., 2012). While escaping from predators or looking for sexual partners, fishes prefer to join larger groups of conspecifics (Agrillo, Dadda & Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-020-00446-1) contains supplementary Bisazza, 2006; Hager & Helfman, 1991;Mehliset al., 2015; material, which is available to authorized users. Potrich et al., 2015). Overall, this evidence shows that animals spontaneously * Bastien S. Lemaire (in the absence of any numerical training) discriminate be- [email protected] tween numerousnesses to deal with various circumstances in their everyday life. Therefore, animal brains seem to be natu- Center for Mind/Brain Sciences, University of Trento, Piazza della rally equipped to use simple numerical cues (Nieder, 2019; Manifaturra 1, 38068 Rovereto, TN, Italy Rugani, 2018; Vallortigara, 2015, 2017). Using operant con- Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, ditioning, several animal species have learned to distinguish Padova, Italy stimuli based on the absolute number of items (Bogale et al., Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, 2011;Bortot etal., 2019; Ditz & Nieder, 2016;Pepperberg, Philadelphia, PA, USA Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 55 2010; Smirnova, Lazareva, & Zorina, 2000; Xia et al., 2001). range (1 vs. 4 and 2 vs. 5; Rugani et al., 2013a). The upper By training animals, numerical achievements can reach a high limit of this kind of numerical discrimination is 3 versus 4. level of abstraction. Beyond numerical discrimination, ani- Nevertheless, this limit can be exceeded by the use of cogni- mals can learn to match symbols to specific numerosities tive strategies as grouping (Rugani, Loconsole, & Regolin, (Biro & Matsuzawa, 2001; Boysen & Berntson, 1989; 2017) and by adding to each object individually distinctive Olthof, Iden, & Roberts, 1997; Pepperberg & Gordon, features, allowing for individual processing (Rugani et al., in 2005), perform simple mathematical operations with those press). symbols (Boysen & Berntson, 1989;Olthof et al., 1997), sym- In 2010, by taking advantage of filial imprinting, it was bolically label subsets of items embedded within heteroge- demonstrated that young domestic chicks, when presented neous arrays (Pepperberg & Gordon, 2005) and master the with two sets of objects, prefer to approach the larger one precursors of a zero-like concept (Howard et al., 2018; (Rugani et al., 2010b). The choice for familiar or novel stimuli Merritt, Rugani & Brannon, 2009; Pepperberg, 1988; in imprinting situations is known to be affected by the sex of Pepperberg & Brezinsky, 1991; Pepperberg & Gordon, the animals. When exposed to familiar and unfamiliar individ- 2005). Sophisticated and abstract numerical concepts can be uals, males and females of domestic chicks behave differently mastered by both primates and birds (Pepperberg, 2009; (Vallortigara & Andrew, 1991; Vallortigara, 1992a, b). Rugani, Vallortigara, & Regolin, 2016b), indicating that bird Females spend more time close to familiar individuals, where- brains, though characterized by a different pallial organization as males spend more time close to unfamiliar ones. (Güntürkün & Bugnyar, 2016), should not be neglected in Furthermore, both sexes pecked more at unfamiliar than at studying high cognition (Gibbs et al., 2008;Matsushima familiar individuals, and overall males pecked more than fe- et al., 2003;Pepperberg, 2017). males. These differences are probably the result of different To investigate the ontogenetic origins of numerical knowl- levels of attachment to conspecifics following imprinting edge, the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) is an ideal animal (Vallortigara, Cailotto, & Zanforlin, 1990). Females seem to model (Rugani, 2018; Versace & Vallortigara, 2015). Unlike develop stronger attachment to their fellows than males studies on adult animals, chicks can be tested very early in life, (McBride & Foenander, 1962; McBride, Parer, & allowing for the discovery of the origins of numerical com- Foenander, 1969; Vallortigara et al., 1990;Workman & prehension. Young chicks can learn to solve different numer- Andrew, 1989). ical problems, ranging from numerical discrimination The aim of this study was to investigate how male and (Rugani, Vallortigara, & Regolin, 2013b)to the useof ordinal female chicks respond to familiar and unfamiliar objects in a cues (Rugani, Regolin, & Vallortigara, 2007), arithmetic cal- number (quantity) discrimination test. Considering the differ- culation (Rugani et al., 2009), comprehension of proportion ent motivation of male and female chicks in approaching their (Rugani, Vallortigara, & Regolin, 2015b), and abstract ratios artificial social companion (Regolin et al., 2005), we expected (Rugani et al., 2016a). a different behavior depending on the sex of the animals. Exploiting chicks’ memories for their imprinting objects Using the original method developed by Rugani et al. allows one to assess whether chicks discriminate between dif- (2010b), we investigated not only how male and female do- ferent numbers of artificial social companions (i.e., objects mestic chicks divided their time in the proximity of a familiar they were exposed to soon after hatching; Rugani, Regolin, versus an unfamiliar number of objects, but also how they & Vallortigara, 2008, 2010b). Filial imprinting is a well- interacted (by social pecking) with familiar and novel objects known phenomenon (Bolhuis, 1991; McCabe, 2019; at test. This new measure allowed us to better understand the Vallortigara & Versace, 2018), allowing the young birds to factors underlying the intrinsic motivation to join the larger learn the characteristics of an object they have been exposed to number of artificial social companions. and to develop a robust social attachment toward it in the first few days of life (Bolhuis, 1991; Yamaguchi et al., 2012). Once imprinted, chicks regard their imprinting objects as so- cial companions (Regolin et al., 2005) and can generalize their Experiment 1 filial behaviors toward similar objects (Bolhuis & Horn, 1992; Versace et al., 2017). Previous studies have exploited chicks’ In this experiment, we applied the experimental procedure of memories for their artificial social companions (object they the first experiment by Rugani et al. (2010b) to test the dis- were reared with) to test their numerical abilities, which range crimination between 1 and 3 imprinting objects in 3-day-old from numerical discrimination (Rugani, Regolin, & domestic chicks. Here we determined the sex of the animal to Vallortigara, 2010b) to proto-arithmetic calculations in the assess any difference in the time spent at test by male versus range of small numbers, up to 3 (Rugani et al., 2009), in the female chicks near the numerically familiar versus numerical- large-number range (6 vs. 9 and 5 vs. 10; Rugani, Regolin & ly novel objects. Moreover, we introduced a novel measure Vallortigara, 2011a) and between the small- and large-number (spontaneous pecking) to assess the level of interaction of 56 Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 each sex based on the scoring of spontaneous pecking toward per cylinder). Half of the chicks were imprinted to one cylin- familiar versus unfamiliar objects at test. der, and the other half were imprinted to three cylinders (Table 1). Food (chick starter crumbs) and water were avail- able ad libitum. Ethics statement After 2 days spent under this rearing conditions (from Tuesday 9:00 am to Thursday 1:00 pm), chicks were individ- The study was performed in compliance with the European ually placed for 6 min in the testing apparatus where they were Union and the Italian laws on the treatment of animals. The free to choose and approach either their familiar numerosity procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the set or an unfamiliar numerosity set. University of Trento and licensed by the Italian Health Ministry (permit number 53/2020). Test Subjects For testing, animals were moved in a room adjacent to the rearing room and placed into the testing apparatus. This The number of chicks required in each group was a priori consisted of a short runway (45 × 20 × 30 cm, Fig. 1B), determined with a power analysis (Champely, 2020)with an illuminated by two LED lamps (12 V). The behavior of each effect size (d) of .65, and an alpha of .05. Results showed that animal was recorded using a Microsoft Life Camera located 20 individuals were required per group to achieve a power of 70 cm above the apparatus. .80. Overall, we used 147 chicks (75 females) of the strain Depending on the experimental condition, two different Ross 308 (Table 1). The eggs were obtained from a commer- sets of stimuli were located at either end of the runway (Fig. cial hatchery (Azienda Agricola Crescenti) and were incubat- 1B). Testing stimuli consisted of same size cylinders (5 cm ed in our laboratory under controlled conditions (37.7 °C and high, 2 cm diameter per cylinder) colored red or blue. Each 40% humidity). Three days before hatching, eggs were moved bird underwent a single test, which could be either an Absolute into opaque black boxes within a hatching chamber at 37.7 °C Discrimination or a Relative Discrimination test. The position and 60% of humidity. of each set in the runway was balanced across animals Soon after hatching, chicks were sexed (this chicken strain (Table 1). exhibits a sexual dimorphism on the wing feathers) and were In the Absolute Discrimination test, chicks were presented singly housed into rectangular cages (22 × 30 × 40 cm, Fig. with one versus three objects, which were identical to the 1A). Cages were illuminated by 30 W fluorescent lights in a imprinting objects (red cylinders, Table 1). In the Relative controlled temperature environment (30 °C). Chicks were Discrimination task, chicks were presented with two compos- reared together with an artificial stimulus (or set of stimuli) ite sets of four objects. One set comprised one red cylinder suspended 1 cm above the floor by a transparent thread in the (familiar object) and three blue cylinders (unfamiliar objects), centre of the cage. By exposing the animals to a set of stimuli while the other set comprised three red cylinders (familiar in their rearing cages, filial imprinting occurred, leading objects) and one blue cylinder (unfamiliar object, Table 1). chicks to develop a strong attachment toward those objects. The positions of the objects within the sets were fixed (such Stimuli consisted of red cylinders (5 cm high, 2 cm diameter as presented in Table 1). In the original study, the Relative Discrimination test was performed to check whether chicks Table 1 Number of animals and sets of stimuli used in the Absolute and would respond to the actual number of familiar objects within Relative Discrimination tests (chicks that did not respond were excluded a set of larger numerosity (composed of unfamiliar objects from the analysis and therefore are not included in this table). In the too). Relative Discrimination test, the position of the objects within the sets was fixed Data analysis To assess the preference toward a set of stimuli, the time spent by the chicks near each set (within a 15-cm area close to it, Fig. 1B) was automatically scored using Ethovision (version 13). A preference for the imprinting numerosity (%) was then calculated using the following formula: Preference for imprinting numerosity time spent close to the familiar imprinting numerosity ¼ x100: time spent close to both sets of stimuli Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 57 Fig. 1 Three-dimensional representation of the rearing cage (A) and the by a transparent thread. Dashed-lines in B delimited the zones (left, center testing apparatus (B). B shows an example of the Absolute and right) in which the time spent by the animal was scored (the zones Discrimination test. The stimuli were suspended 1 cm above the floor were defined computationally using Ethovision) Post hoc Tukey tests were performed when required using A value higher than 50% indicated a preference for the fa- miliar imprinting numerosity (if imprinted with 1, a preference Bonferroni’scorrection. All the statistical analyses were performed using RStudio for 1). A value lower than 50% indicated a preference for the unfamiliar numerosity set (if imprinted with 1, a preference for v1.1 (RStudio Team, 2015) with the following packages: goftest (Faraway et al., 2019), nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2020), 3). A score of 50% indicated no preference (chance level). As pecking is a behavior expressed during social exploration lme (Bates et al., 2015), tidyr (Wickham & Lionel, 2020), and recognition in domestic chicks (Gottier, 1968;Nicol, 2015; plyr (Wickham, 2011), dplyr (Wickham et al., 2020), reshape (Wickham, 2007), lsr (Navarro, 2015), ggplot2 (Wickham, Schjelderup-Ebbe, 1922; Vallortigara, 1992a), we also scored the number of pecks assigned by the animals to each stimulus 2016), pwr (Champely, 2020). within each set. Manual scoring was made by a scorer blind to the experimental conditions. The coding reliability of the pecks Results was assessed by re-coding 21 chicks randomly selected (Pearson’s correlation test showed a high correlation between Absolute Discrimination the two codings, r = 0.99). Pecking analysis was performed once the videorecordings were already archived. Preference for the imprinting numerosity The results are Unfortunately, while uploading the videos, a folder got showninFig. 2A. There was a significant effect of Sex corrupted and prevented us from coding the pecking behaviour of 21 chicks (12 females) in the Relative Discrimination test. (F(1, 66) = 6.73, p < 0.05), but not of Imprinting Numerosity (F(1, 66) = 0.20, p = 0.66) or interaction (F(1, Chicks that did not respond (one female and two males) were removed from the analysis as they did not score any 66) = 1.69, p =0.20). Overall, the preference for the imprinting numerosity set preference during the entire testing duration. was significantly different from chance level for males (t(33) = 2.50, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 0.42, Bonferroni correction), but Statistical analysis not for females (t(35) = -0.54, p = 0.59, Cohen’sd=0.090, Bonferroni correction). Males spent on average 66% (± 5.89 To determine whether animals showed preferences for the sets SEM) of their time close to the imprinting numerosity set, while females spent on average 47% (± 6.42 SEM) of their of objects differing across Sex (female, male) and Imprinting time close to it. Numerosity (1, 3), we performed an ANOVA for each dis- crimination task (Absolute, Relative). To meet parametric as- Pecks The results are shown in Fig. 2B. When imprinted with 1, sumptions, we arcsine-transformed the data. To check wheth- er chicks had a significant preference for the imprinting there was a significant effect of Object (F(3, 132) = 6.46, p < 0.01) and of the interaction between Object and Sex (F(3, 132) numerosity, we performed two-tailed one-sample t-tests against chance level (50%). = 6.36, p < 0.01) but a non-significant effect of Sex (F(1, 132) = 0.86, p = 0.36). While females pecked similarly at each object, To determine whether animals were pecking at the stimuli (each cylinder within a set of stimuli) differently across Sex the post hoc analysis revealed that males pecked significantly more at the single object in comparison to the central (t(132) = (female, male), we performed an ANOVA for each discrimina- tion task (Absolute, Relative) and Imprinting Numerosity (1, 3). 4.10, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 1.41) and right-most (t(132) = 3.50, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = 1.19) objects within the set of three. To meet parametric assumptions, we log-transformed the data. 58 Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 Fig. 2 Graph (A) represents the time spent by the chicks during testing (females in red and males in light-blue) and Imprinting Numerosity (* p close to their imprinting numerosity between Imprinting Numerosity and < 0.05; ** p < 0.01). Light blue asterisks show the statistical difference in Sex (p < 0.05, *). Graph (B) represents the number of pecks made toward males; black asterisks show the statistical differences in both males and each stimulus composing the different set of stimuli between Sex females When imprinted with 3, there was a significant effect of Sex significant effect of Sex (F(1, 70) = 0.19, p =0.67) norof the (F(1, 132) = 4.12, p < 0.05) and Object (F(3, 132) = 3.99, p < interaction (F(1, 70) = 0.00, p =0.99). 0.01), but no interaction (F(3, 132) = 0.30, p = 0.82). As revealed Overall, the preference for the imprinting numerosity set by the ANOVA, males (mean = 9.90, SEM = 2.27) pecked was significantly different from chance level for the chicks significantly more than females (mean = 5.31, SEM = 1.25) at imprinted with 1 (t(35) = -4.74, p <0.001, Cohen’sd= the objects, a difference that seems to mainly be driven by the 0.79, Bonferroni correction) and 3 (t(37) = 5.32, p < 0.001, number of pecks made at the left-most object within the set of Cohen’s d = 0.86, Bonferroni correction). Nevertheless, the three. Males pecked on average 15 times (SEM = 4.79) at it, preferences between these two groups were the opposite. while females pecked on average five times (SEM = 1.76) at it. Chicks imprinted with 1 spent on average 23% (±5.64 SEM) Overall, the post hoc analysis revealed that chicks pecked of their time close to the imprinting numerosity set while significantly more at the single stimulus in comparison to the chicks imprinted with 3 spent on average 79% (±5.36 SEM) central object composing the set of three stimuli (t(132) = close to it. This indicates that, independently of rearing con- 3.05, p < 0.05, Cohen’s d =0.73). ditions and sex, chicks approached the larger set of familiar objects. Relative Discrimination Pecks The results are shown in Fig. 3B. When imprinted on 1 Preference for the imprinting numerosity The results are or 3, there was a significant effect of Object (imprinted with 1: showninFig. 3A. There was a significant effect of F(7, 184) = 11.48, p < 0.001; imprinted with 3: F(7, 208) = Imprinting Numerosity (F(1, 70) = 53.16, p < 0.001) but no 7.75, p < 0.001), but not a significant effect of Sex (imprinted Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 59 Fig. 3 (A) The time spent by the chicks during testing close to the object different set of stimuli between Imprinting Numerosity (females and set containing the number of familiar objects experienced during males grouped; *** p < 0.001). Black asterisks show the statistical imprinting between Imprinting Numerosity and Sex (p < 0.001, ***). differences in both males and females (B) The number of pecks made toward each stimulus composing the with 1: F(1, 184) = 0.01, p = 0.92; imprinted with 3: F(1, 208) motivation of male and female birds in exploring a novel =1.10, p = 0.30) or of the interaction (imprinted with 1: F(7, numerousness of familiar objects. Males seemed more in- 184) = 0.36, p = 0.93; imprinted with 3: F(7, 208) = 0.71, p = clined to spend time close to the familiar numerousness, 0.66). while females seemed equally motivated in joining the fa- No matter whether they had been imprinted on 1 or 3, the miliar group or exploring the novel one. When tested in the post hoc analysis revealed that chicks pecked significantly presence of familiar and unfamiliar objects (Relative more at the single blue object in the set in comparison to all Discrimination), both sexes used the relative numerical other objects (statistics are detailed in the Online information available concerning the subset of familiar ob- Supplemental Material table). jects present within each set and chose to associate with the larger set of familiar objects as described in the original study by Rugani et al. (2010a, b). Thepresenceofnovel Experiment 2 objects seems to play a relevant role in the choice to ap- proach the larger number of familiar objects, or in avoiding In the first experiment, chicks behaved differently in the the larger number of unfamiliar ones. Support for the latter two discrimination tests. When only exposed to familiar hypothesis comes from the analyses of the pecks, which objectsattest(Absolute Discrimination), males consistently highlight an increased number of pecks at the approached the familiar numerousness, whereas females single novel object in the group comprising three familiar did not show any preference. This may indicate a different objects and an unfamiliar one. 60 Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 To investigate the importance of unfamiliarity, we conduct- 1.96, p = 0.12). As revealed by the ANOVA, males were ed a second experiment in which we manipulated the degree pecking (mean = 8.80, SEM = 1.40) significantly more than of unfamiliarity. females (mean = 6.21, SEM = 1.39) at the objects. Furthermore, the post hoc analysis revealed that males pecked significantly more at the left-most object (mean = Methods 11.16, SEM = 2.39) in the set of three objects in comparison to the single object (mean = 8.21, SEM = 4.67; t(144) = -3.45, The general procedure was the same as in the first experiment. p < 0.05, Cohen’s d = -1.12). The opposite tendency was The same number of objects as in the Absolute Discrimination observed in females although not revealed by the post hoc was used (Table 2), but this time we slightly modified the analysis as the variability of the pecks to the single object appearance of the stimuli during testing. Chicks at test were seemed quite high (mean = 13.42, SEM = 5.84) in comparison offered a choice between one or three objects, but a small to that of the left-most object within the set of three objects yellow dot (diameter of 5 mm) was added on the central object (mean = 5.79, SEM = 1.61). of the set corresponding to the original imprinting numerosity When imprinted with 3, there was a significant effect of (Table 2). For the chicks that had been imprinted to one object, Object (F(3, 144) = 6.92, p < 0.001) but no significant effects the yellow dot was placed on the single object during testing. of Sex (F(1, 144) = 0.46, p = 0.50) nor of the interaction (F(3, For the chicks that had been imprinted to three objects, the 144) = 0.23, p =0.87). yellow dot was placed on the central object composing the set The post hoc analysis revealed that chicks pecked signifi- of three during testing. cantly more at the single stimulus in comparison to the left- We used 79 chicks (40 females) of the strain Ross 308 most one (t(144) = 3.48, p < 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.80) or to the (Table 2). Chicks that did not respond (two females and one right-most one (t(144) = 4.10, p <0.001, Cohen’s d =0.94) in male) were removed from the analysis as they did not score the set of three objects . any preference during the entire testing duration. Results General discussion Preference for the imprinting numerosity The results are Number cognition in birds has been widely investigated. showninFig. 4A. There was a significant effect of Studies performed by Pepperberg on Alex the parrot illustrat- Imprinting Numerosity (F(1, 72) = 7.43, p < 0.01) but no ed the impressive numerical competences owned by avian significant effects of Sex (F(1, 72) = 0.075, p = 0.79) or of species (Pepperberg, 2009). Alex was able to quantify up to the interaction (F(1, 72) = 1.87, p =0.18). six-item sets using English labels with an accuracy of 80%, Overall, the preference for the imprinting numerosity was and remaining unaffected by array quantity, mass, or contour significantly different from chance level for the chicks (Pepperberg, 1994; Pepperberg & Gordon, 2005). The imprinted to 1 (t(37) = -5.86, p <0.001, Cohen’s d =0.97, achievement of a high level of abstraction in numerical com- Bonferroni correction) but not for the chicks imprinted to 3 prehension suggests that birds may naturally deal with numer- (t(37) = -0.83, p = 0.41, Cohen’s d = 0.14, Bonferroni correc- ical information in everyday life. In our study, instead of fo- tion). Chicks imprinted to 1 spent on average 20% (±4.98 cusing on the symbolic use of numbers as Pepperberg did, we SEM) of their time close to the familiar numerosity, while studied a much simpler numerical ability (numerousness dis- chicks imprinted to 3 spent on average 44% (±6.62 SEM) crimination), exploiting a paradigm that allows one to test close to it. numerical comprehension in the absence of any numerical training. This allows for the understanding of how birds can Pecks The results are shown in Fig. 4B. Whenimprintedwith spontaneously deal with numerical cues. 1, there was a significant effect of Sex (F(1, 144) = 5.51, p < As in previous studies (Rugani et al., 2008, 2010b), our 0.05) and an interaction between Object and Sex (F(3, 144) = results showed that chicks could use numerical/quantity infor- 4.23, p < 0.01), but no significant effect of Object (F(3, 144) = mation to discriminate between different sets of objects. As we did not control for the role of continuous physical vari- Table 2 Number of animals and sets of stimuli used in the second ables, we cannot disentangle whether chicks used numerical experiment (chicks that did not respond were excluded from the and/or continuous quantities to discriminate. However, previ- analysis and therefore are not included in this table) ous work has shown that chicks, depending on the available cues, could use both sources of information (Rugani et al., 2010b). Moreover, several sources of supporting evidence points to the existence of a general magnitude system in hu- man and non-human species that comprises both discrete Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 61 Fig. 4 Graph (A) represents the time spent by the chicks during testing between Sex (females in red and males in light-blue) and Imprinting close to their imprinting numerosity set between Imprinting Numerosity Numerosity (* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001). Light blue asterisk and Sex (*** p < 0.015). Graph (B) represents the number of pecks shows the statistical difference in males; black asterisks show the statis- assigned to each stimulus composing the different sets of stimuli tical differences for both males and females (countable) and continue quantities (Bortot, Stancher & and had a strong preference to associate with the set composed Vallortigara, 2020; De Corte, Navarro & Wasserman, 2017; of more familiar objects (three red objects and one blue ob- Di Giorgio et al., 2019;Gallistel, 1989;Merritt, Casasanto, & ject). Motivational differences may support chicks’ tendency Brannon, 2010;Walsh, 2003). to approach the larger group of social companions: the larger In our study, we focused on whether males and females group can guarantee more protection toward potential preda- used numerical/quantities cues similarly. We found that males tors, higher level of social interaction, a richer environment, and females make different use of numerical cues depending and in the natural situation more heat (Pulliam, 1973;Roberts, on the context (familiar, in the Absolute Discrimination task, 1996). Furthermore, the experimental procedure used in our or familiar and unfamiliar in the Relative Discrimination task). study exploits the chick’s memory for its imprinting object (or The Absolute Discrimination task showed that males prefer set of objects). As a result of filial imprinting, an increase in to associate with the familiar numerosity set, while females fear is observed when exposed to novel stimuli (Bolhuis, 1991). This leads the young animals to avoid proximity with showed no preference. This may indicate that in a novel envi- ronment, as the testing apparatus was, males are motivated to novel objects, which therefore could explain why chicks tend approach the familiar numerosity sets, while females are to associate with the set composed of the largest number of equally motivated to explore the two sets. The Relative familiar objects and the fewest number of unfamiliar objects. Discrimination task revealed a completely different pattern The number of pecks here is not to be interpreted as a of results, but this time more similar to what Rugani et al. feeding behavior. Instead, it likely reflects a social, either (2010b) described initially. Males and females behaved alike affiliative or aggressive, behavior toward familiar or 62 Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 unfamiliar objects (Vallortigara, 1992a), providing inter- 1992a, b). Hence, it is possible that chicks used esting cues as to the possible nature of sex differences. numerical/quantitative information available to determine While affiliative pecks are usually equally distributed which object was more likely to be unfamiliar. across time among familiar individuals, aggressive pecks A similar interpretation can be made by looking at the are reiterated toward unfamiliar individuals/objects results obtained in the Relative Discrimination task. Chicks (Schjelderup-Ebbe, 1935). Pecking behavior may, there- repeatedly pecked at the blue object incorporating the larger fore, reveal lack of recognition when intensively exhibited set of familiar objects (three red objects), demonstrating that toward an individual (Guhl & Ortman, 1953; Vallortigara, they recognized the blue object as being unfamiliar within a 1992a, b). In the Absolute Discrimination task, males familiar set. pecked more at the familiar numerosity set. This measure Our results suggest that male and female domestic correlates with the time spent near the familiar numerous- chicks use numerical/quantity information slightly differ- ness and shows a preference of male chicks to interact ently depending on the familiarity of the objects. In a more with the familiar set rather than with the novel familiar context, males tend to use numerical/quantity in- one. A peculiar behavior emerged from the analysis of formation to discriminate between two sets of familiar the distribution of pecks toward the array of three objects: objects, while females focus on each familiar object indi- males seemed to peck more at the left object than at the vidually. Interestingly, in a context composed of unfamil- central or at the right one. This left-sided preference could iar objects, males and females expressed similar behaviors be related to a general bias in the allocation of spatial and chose to associate with the set containing less unfa- attention (Diekamp et al., 2005; Regolin, 2006; Rugani miliar objects. In the previous study (Rugani et al., et al., 2011b). Day-old domestic chicks, in fact, associate 2010b), no difference was found between the Absolute numbers with space in different contexts. Chicks trained and Relative Discrimination tasks. Therefore, the authors to respond to a certain numerical value spontaneously concluded that chicks mainly use the number of familiar associated a smaller number with the left side and a larger objects to discriminate between two sets of stimuli and number with the right side of space (Rugani et al., 2020, that they choose to associate with the larger set of familiar 2015a). Chicks, trained to identify a target element (e.g., objects. Our data instead support the idea that chicks the fourth) in a sagitally oriented series of identical ele- mainly rely on the number of unfamiliar objects instead ments, when required to react to an identical series but of the number of familiar ones. This difference could be rotated by 90°, identified most often the left target than possibly explained by the colors used: yellow and light the right one (Rugani et al., 2010a, 2015a). A lateral bias pink in the study by Rugani et al. (2010b), versus red has also been found in a numerical task which required to and blue in the present study. The relevance of the unfa- discriminate between two groups of artificial social com- miliar object may have been emphasized using novel blue panions. Female chicks were reared with a set of identical objects instead of light pink ones. Even if different strat- objects. At test, the objects disappeared one at a time egies seem to be at the basis of the current study (avoid behind one of two identical screens, one on the left and the larger number of unfamiliar objects) and in the previ- one on the right. On a free-choice test, chicks showed a ous study (approach the larger number of familiar objects), preference for the larger group. Nevertheless, their perfor- nevertheless both studies converge in demonstrating that mance was higher when the larger group was hidden on day-old chicks do discriminate numerousness in the ab- the right side (Rugani, Rosa Salva, & Regolin, 2014). sence of any numerical training. Differences in strains This evidence suggests that also in a spontaneous search should also be considered. for social companions a tendency can emerge to associate To better understand to which degree unfamiliarity influ- numbers and space. The latter evidence is also in line with ences chicks’ behaviors, we conducted a second experiment. our new findings in which male chicks tend to explore the The task in this second experiment shared properties with the smaller numbers of social companions on the left side. Absolute and Relative Discrimination tasks. During testing, Going back to our current study, females, in contrast, chicks were offered a choice between a set of one or three visited and pecked at all objects individually, which can objects (suchasinthe Absolute condition). However, we probably explain why they did not express any preference slightly changed the appearance of the familiar numerosity in the time spent analysis (females were at chance-level). set so that it neither appeared completely familiar nor Curiously, when imprinted with the set composed of three completely unfamiliar, by adding a small yellow dot on one objects, both sexes significantly pecked more at the single of the objects of the familiar numerosity set (for the stimulus as if they were treating it as a less familiar indi- discriminability of imprinting object based on individual vidual. Indeed, pecking can also demonstrate a lack of features depicted on them, see Fontanari et al., 2011). recognition when it is repeatedly directed toward a spe- In the second experiment, chicks tended to explore the cific individual (Guhl & Ortman, 1953; Vallortigara, unfamiliar numerosity set. This demonstrates that even a slight Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 63 change in the appearance of one object influences the chick’s a context, using numerical abilities would be an efficient strat- decision to associate with either set. Although no sex differ- egy, as has been shown to occur in several other species ence was observed in terms of time spent close to either set, (Benson-Amram et al., 2011; Bonanni et al., 2011; Cassidy the pecking analysis revealed that males and females behaved et al., 2015; McComb, Packer, & Pusey, 1994;Van Belle & differently. When imprinted with one, females pecked more at Scarry, 2015; Wilson et al., 2012). the single object (with the yellow dot) than at the other objects In conclusion, our results show that young and almost (set of three, where they spent 72% of their time). As in the naïve domestic chicks rely on numerical information to make Absolute Discrimination task, females seem to focus more on social decisions. They first discriminate between familiar ver- individual recognition, which suggests that they do not treat sus unfamiliar objects based on their perceptual features, and the single stimulus (with the yellow dot) as entirely unfamiliar. then they estimate the numerousness of both sets to avoid the In contrast, males completely avoided the single stimulus larger number of unfamiliar objects. Moreover, the degree of (with the yellow dot) and pecked more at the set of three. novelty of the unfamiliar objects seems to correlate with the The results of this second experiment appear to be in agree- avoidance of the unfamiliar set. Numerousness is, therefore, a ment with the study by Rugani, Regolin and Vallortigara relevant information animals can spontaneously use in a social (2010b) and with our first experiment, suggesting that chicks context to optimize their fitness. firstly discriminate between familiar and unfamiliar objects Acknowledgements We are grateful to Elif Karakoç for helping to test before making a decision based on their numerousness. the animals, and to Nicholas Zampiero and Chiara Eccher for assisting in Our study provides additional information concerning the the coding of the chicks’ behaviors. use of numerical or quantitative information by young domes- This project was supported by funding from the European Research tic chicks in the specific context of filial imprinting. Taken Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (grant agreement No. 833504 SPANUMBRA) to together, our results confirm that chicks can use numerousness GV. The project also received funding from the European Union’s to discriminate between different set of objects. It also dem- Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie onstrates that male and female domestic chicks do not always Sklodowska-Curie (grant agreement No. 795242 SNANeB) to RR; and use available numerical cues similarly, and that, instead, they from a PRIN 2017 ERC-SH4–A (2017PSRHPZ) to LR and GV. might prefer to use different strategies depending on the fa- Funding Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di miliarity of the objects. Overall, females seem more flexible in Trento within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. the use of numerical/quantitative cues depending on the con- text. In a familiar context, females perform individual recog- Data availability The datasets (.csv) are available on Fig Share (10.6084/ nition rather than using numerical/quantitative information to m9.figshare.12336140). make a decision. In an environment composed of familiar as Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons well as unfamiliar objects, females used numerical informa- Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, tion to discriminate between two sets of objects. In contrast, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as males tend to rely upon numerical information either by ap- long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the proaching the familiar numerosity set when exposed to famil- source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article iar objects or the larger set of familiar objects when exposed to are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated sets of familiar and unfamiliar objects. otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the Likely the sex difference we observed may derive from the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not natural history of feral birds. Adult fowls are organized in permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a groups comprising a dominant rooster and many hens copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. (Queiroz & Cromberg, 2006). Males are more solitary as they spend most of their time maintaining and patrolling their ter- ritory, whereas females tend to live in strict hierarchies that they develop and maintain through time (Gottier, 1968; References McBride & Foenander, 1962; McBride et al., 1969; Schjelderup-Ebbe, 1922). Such organization may favor the Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., & Bisazza, A. (2006). Quantity discrimination in prevalence of gregarious and affiliative behaviors in females female mosquitofish. Animal Cognition, 10(1), 63–70. https://doi. (Cailotto, Vallortigara, & Zanforlin, 1989; Vallortigara et al., org/10.1007/s10071-006-0036-5 1990;Vallortigara, 1992b; Workman & Andrew, 1989), as Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, well as greater use of specific abilities such as transitive infer- 67(1). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 ence (Daisley, Vallortigara, & Regolin, 2010) favoring indi- Benson-Amram, S., Heinen, V. K., Dryer, S. L., & Holekamp, K. E. vidual recognition. Given their roles of chaperone, males (2011). Numerical assessment and individual call discrimination might overlook individual recognition and instead rely on by wild spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Animal Behaviour, cues helping them to assess potential threats promptly. In such 82(4), 743–752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.004 64 Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 Biro, D., & Matsuzawa, T. (2001). Use of numerical symbols by the Fontanari, L., Rugani, R., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2011). Object chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): Cardinals, ordinals, and the introduc- individuation in 3-day-old chicks: use of property and spatiotempo- tion of zero. Animal Cognition, 4(3–4), 193–199. https://doi.org/10. ral information. Developmental Science, 14(5), 1235–1244. https:// 1007/s100710100086 doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01074.x Bogale, B. A., Aoyama, M., & Sugita, S. (2014). Spontaneous discrim- Gallistel, C. R. (1989). Animal cognition: the representation of space, ination of food quantities in the jungle crow, Corvus time and number. Annual Review of Psychology, 40(1), 155–189. macrorhynchos. Animal Behaviour, 94,73–78. https://doi.org/10. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.40.020189.001103 1016/j.anbehav.2014.05.012 Gazzola, A., Vallortigara, G., & Pellitteri-Rosa, D. (2018). Continuous Bogale, B. A., Kamata, N., Mioko, K., & Sugita, S. (2011). Quantity and discrete quantity discrimination in tortoises. Biology Letters, discrimination in jungle crows, Corvus macrorhynchos. Animal 14(12), 20180649. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2018.0649 Behaviour, 82(4), 635–641. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav. Gibbs, M. E., Gibbs, C., Csillag, A., Matsushima, T., Rogers, L., & 2011.05.025 Rostas, J. (2008). Brain mechanisms, cognition and behaviour in Bolhuis, J. J. (1991). Mechanisms of avian imprinting: a review. birds. Brain Research Bulletin, 76(3), 167–169. https://doi.org/10. Biological Reviews, 66(4), 303–345. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. 1016/j.brainresbull.2008.03.012 1469-185X.1991.tb01145.x Gottier, R. F. (1968). The dominance-submission hierarchy in the social Bolhuis, Johan J., & Horn, G. (1992). Generalization of learned prefer- behavior of the domestic chicken. The Journal of Genetic ences in filial imprinting. Animal Behaviour, 44(November 1992), Psychology, 112(2), 205–226. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325. 185–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80773-0 1968.10533796 Bonanni, R., Natoli, E., Cafazzo, S., & Valsecchi, P. (2011). Free-ranging Guhl, A. M., & Ortman, L. L. (1953). Visual patterns in the recognition of dogs assess the quantity of opponents in intergroup conflicts. Animal individuals among chickens. The Condor, 55(6), 287–298. https:// Cognition, 14(1), 103–115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-010- doi.org/10.2307/1365008 0348-3 Güntürkün, O., & Bugnyar, T. (2016). Cognition without cortex. Trends Bortot, M., Agrillo, C., Avarguès-Weber, A.,Bisazza,A., Miletto in Cognitive Sciences, 20(4), 291–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Petrazzini, M. E., & Giurfa, M. (2019). Honeybees use absolute tics.2016.02.001 rather than relative numerosity in number discrimination. Biology Hager, M. C., & Helfman, G. S. (1991). Safety in numbers: shoal size Letters, 15(6), 20190138. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2019.0138 choice by minnows under predatory threat. Behavioral Ecology and Bortot, M., Stancher, G., & Vallortigara, G. (2020). Transfer from num- Sociobiology, 29(4), 271–276. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00163984 ber to size reveals abstract coding of magnitude in honeybees. Hauser, M. D., Carey, S., & Hauser, L. B. (2000). Spontaneous number IScience, 101122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2020.101122 representation in semi–free–ranging rhesus monkeys. Proceedings Boysen, S. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1989). Numerical competence in a of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 267(1445), 829–833. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1078 103(1), 23–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.103.1.23 Howard, S. R., Avarguès-Weber, A., Garcia, J. E., Greentree, A. D., & Cailotto, M., Vallortigara, G., & Zanforlin, M. (1989). Sex differences in Dyer, A. G. (2018). Numerical ordering of zero in honey bees. the response to social stimuli in young chicks. Ethology Ecology & Science, 360(6393), 1124–1126. https://doi.org/10.1126/science. Evolution, 1(4), 323–327. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927014.1989. aar4975 9525502 Hunt, S., Low, J., & Burns, K. C. (2008). Adaptive numerical competen- Cassidy, K. A., MacNulty, D. R., Stahler, D. R., Smith, D. W., & Mech, cy in a food-hoarding songbird. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: L. D. (2015). Group composition effects on aggressive interpack Biological Sciences, 275(1649), 2373–2379. https://doi.org/10. interactions of gray wolves in Yellowstone National Park. 1098/rspb.2008.0702 Behavioral Ecology, 26(5), 1352–1360. https://doi.org/10.1093/ Lucon-Xiccato, T., Miletto Petrazzini, M. E., Agrillo, C., & Bisazza, A. beheco/arv081 (2015). Guppies discriminate between two quantities of food items Champely, S. (2020). pwr: basic functions for power analysis. Retrieved but prioritize item size over total amount. Animal Behaviour, 107, from https://cran.r-project.org/package=pwr 183–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.06.019 Daisley, J. N., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, L. (2010). Logic in an asym- Matsushima, T., Izawa, E.-I., Aoki, N., & Yanagihara, S. (2003). The metrical (social) brain: transitive inference in the young domestic mind through chick eyes : memory, cognition and anticipation. chick. Social Neuroscience, 5(3), 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Zoological Science, 20(4), 395–408. https://doi.org/10.2108/zsj.20. 17470910903529795 395 De Corte, B. J., Navarro, V. M., & Wasserman, E. A. (2017). Non- McBride, G., & Foenander, F. (1962). Territorial behaviour in flocks of cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons. Current domestic fowls. Nature, 194(4823), 102–102. https://doi.org/10. Biology, 27(23), R1264–R1265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017. 1038/194102a0 10.029 McBride, G., Parer, I. P., & Foenander, F. (1969). The social organization Di Giorgio, E., Lunghi, M., Rugani, R., Regolin, L., Dalla Barba, B., and behaviour of the feral domestic fowl. Animal Behaviour Vallortigara, G., & Simion, F. (2019). A mental number line in Monographs, 2,125–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0066-1856(69) human newborns. Developmental Science, 22(6). https://doi.org/ 80003-8 10.1111/desc.12801 McCabe, B. J. (2019). Visual imprinting in birds: behavior, models, and Diekamp, B., Regolin, L., Güntürkün, O., & Vallortigara, G. (2005). A neural mechanisms. Frontiers in Physiology, 10. https://doi.org/10. left-sided visuospatial bias in birds. Current Biology, 15(10), R372– 3389/fphys.2019.00658 R373. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2005.05.017 McComb, K., Packer, C., & Pusey, A. (1994). Roaring and numerical Ditz, H. M., & Nieder, A. (2016). Numerosity representations in crows assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo. obey the Weber–Fechner law. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Animal Behaviour, 47(2), 379–387. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe. Biological Sciences, 283(1827), 20160083. https://doi.org/10.1098/ 1994.1052 rspb.2016.0083 Mehlis, M., Thünken, T., Bakker, T. C. M., & Frommen, J. G. (2015). Faraway, J., Marsaglia, G., Marsaglia, J., & Baddeley, A. (2019). goftest: Quantification acuity in spontaneous shoaling decisions of three- classical goodness-of-fit tests for univariate distributions. Retrieved spined sticklebacks. Animal Cognition, 18(5), 1125–1131. https:// from https://cran.r-project.org/package=goftest doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0884-y Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 65 Merritt, D. J., Rugani, R., & Brannon, E. M. (2009). Empty sets as part of Gallus gallus domesticus. Animal Behaviour, 70(4), 855–864. the numerical continuum: Conceptual precursors to the zero concept https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.01.014 in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, Roberts, G. (1996). Why individual vigilance declines as group size in- 138(2), 258–269. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015231 creases. Animal Behaviour, 51(5), 1077–1086. https://doi.org/10. Merritt, Dustin J., Casasanto, D., & Brannon, E. M. (2010). Do monkeys 1006/anbe.1996.0109 think in metaphors? Representations of space and time in monkeys Rodríguez, R. L., Briceño, R. D., Briceño-Aguilar, E., & Höbel, G. and humans. Cognition, 117(2), 191–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. (2015). Nephila clavipes spiders (Araneae nephilidae) keep track cognition.2010.08.011 of captured prey counts: testing for a sense of numerosity in an Navarro, D. (2015). Learning statistics with R: A tutorial for psychology orb-weaver. Animal Cognition, 18(1), 307–314. https://doi.org/10. students and other beginners. Retrieved from http://ua.edu.au/ccs/ 1007/s10071-014-0801-9 teaching/lsr RStudio Team. (2015). RStudio: integrated development for R. Retrieved Nicol, C. J. (2015). The behavioural biology of chickens. (C. J. Nicol, from http://www.rstudio.com Ed.). Wallingford: CABI. https://doi.org/10.1079/9781780642499. Rugani, R. (2018). Towards numerical cognition’s origin: insights from day-old domestic chicks. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Nieder, A. (2019). A brain for numbers: the biology of the number Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1740), 20160509. https://doi. instinct. MIT press. org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0509 Nieder, A. (2020). The adaptive value of numerical competence. Trends Rugani, R., Cavazzana, A., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, L. (2013a). One, in Ecology & Evolution https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2020.02.009 two, three, four, or is there something more? Numerical discrimina- tion in day-old domestic chicks. Animal Cognition, 16(4), 557–564. Olthof, A., Iden, C. M., & Roberts, W. A. (1997). Judgments of ordinality https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0593-8 and summation of number symbols by squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Rugani, R., Fontanari, L., Simoni, E., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. Processes, 23(3), 325–339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.23. (2009). Arithmetic in newborn chicks. Proceedings of the Royal 3.325 Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1666), 2451–2460. https://doi. Pepperberg, I. M. (1988). Comprehension of “abscence” by an African org/10.1098/rspb.2009.0044 grey parrot: learning with respect to questions of same/different. Rugani, R., Kelly, D. M., Szelest, I., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50(3), 553–564. (2010a). Is it only humans that count from left to right? Biology https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1988.50-553 Letters, 6(3), 290–292. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0960 Pepperberg, I. M. (1994). Numerical competence in an African gray Rugani, R., Loconsole, M., & Regolin, L. (2017). A strategy to improve parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, arithmetical performance in four day-old domestic chicks (Gallus 108(1), 36–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.36 gallus). Scientific Reports, 7(1), 13900. https://doi.org/10.1038/ Pepperberg, I. M. (2009). The Alex studies. Harvard University Press. s41598-017-13677-6 https://doi.org/10.2307/j.ctvk12qc1 Rugani, R., Loconsole, M.,Simion, F.,&Regolin,L.(in press). Pepperberg, I. M. (2010). Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in Individually distinctive features facilitate numerical discrimination the African grey parrot: labeling of cardinal sets. Ethology, 75(1), of sets of objects in domestic chicks. Scientific Reports. 37–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1987.tb00641.x Rugani, R., McCrink, K., de Hevia, M.-D., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, Pepperberg, I. M. (2017). Birdbrains’ should not be ignored in studying L. (2016a). Ratio abstraction over discrete magnitudes by newly the evolution of g. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40,e216. https:// hatched domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Scientific Reports, 6(1), doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16001758 30114. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep30114 Pepperberg, I. M., & Brezinsky, M. V. (1991). Acquisition of a relative Rugani, R., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2007). Rudimental numerical class concept by an African gray parrot (Psittacus erithacus): dis- competence in 5-day-old domestic chicks (Gallus gallus): identifi- criminations based on relative size. Journal of Comparative cation of ordinal position. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Psychology, 105(3), 286–294. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036. Animal Behavior Processes, 33(1), 21–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/ 105.3.286 0097-7403.33.1.21 Pepperberg, I. M., & Gordon, J. D. (2005). Number comprehension by a Rugani, R., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2008). Discrimination of grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus), including a zero-like concept. small numerosities in young chicks. Journal of Experimental Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(2), 197–209. https://doi. Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 34(3), 388–399. https:// org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.197 doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.34.3.388 Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Core, T. (2020). Rugani, R., Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2011a). Summation of large Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. Retrieved from https:// numerousness by newborn chicks. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. cran.r-project.org/package=nlme https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00179 Potrich, D., Sovrano, V. A., Stancher, G., & Vallortigara, G. (2015). Rugani, R., Rosa Salva, O., & Regolin, L. (2014). Lateralized mecha- Quantity discrimination by zebrafish (Danio rerio). Journal of nisms for encoding of object. Behavioral evidence from an animal Comparative Psychology, 129(4), 388–393. https://doi.org/10. model: the domestic chick (Gallus gallus). Frontiers in Psychology, 1037/com0000012 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00150 Pulliam, H. R. (1973). On the advantages of flocking. Journal of Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., & Regolin, L. (2015a). Number- Theoretical Biology, 38(2), 419–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022- space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans' mental 5193(73)90184-7 number line. Science, 347(6221), 534–536. https://doi.org/10. Queiroz, S. A., & Cromberg, V. U. (2006). Aggressive behavior in the 1126/science.aaa1379 genus Gallus sp. Revista Brasileira de Ciência Avícola, 8(1), 1–14. Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., & Regolin, L. (2020). Numerical https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-635X2006000100001 magnitude, rather than individual bias, explains spatial numerical Regolin, L. (2006). The case of the line-bisection: when both humans and association in newborn chicks. ELife, 9. https://doi.org/10.7554/ chickens wander left. Cortex, 42(1), 101–103. https://doi.org/10. eLife.54662 1016/S0010-9452(08)70330-7 Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, L. (2013b). Numerical abstrac- Regolin, L., Rugani, R., Pagni, P., & Vallortigara, G. (2005). Delayed tion in young domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). PLoS ONE, 8(6), search for social and nonsocial goals by young domestic chicks, e65262. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065262 66 Learn Behav (2021) 49:54–66 Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, L. (2015b). The use of propor- Van Belle, S., & Scarry, C. J. (2015). Individual participation in inter- tion by young domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Animal Cognition, group contests is mediated by numerical assessment strategies in 18(3), 605–616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0829-x black howler and tufted capuchin monkeys. Philosophical Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., & Regolin, L. (2016b). Mapping number to Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, space in the two hemispheres of the avian brain. Neurobiology of 370(1683), 20150007. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0007 Learning and Memory, 133,13–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm. Versace, E., Spierings, M. J., Caffini, M., en Cate, C., & Vallortigara, G. 2016.05.010 (2017). Spontaneous generalization of abstract multimodal patterns Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Vallini, B., & Regolin, L. (2011b). in young domestic chicks. Animal Cognition, 20(3), 521–529. Asymmetrical number-space mapping in the avian brain. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-017-1079-5 Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95(3), 231–238. https:// Versace, E., & Vallortigara, G. (2015). Origins of Knowledge: insights doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2010.11.012 from precocial species. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9. Rugani, R, Regolin, L., & Vallortigara, G. (2010b). Imprinted numbers: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00338 newborn chicks' sensitivity to number vs. continuous extent of ob- Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of jects they have been reared with. Developmental Science, 13(5), time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483– 790–797. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00936.x 488. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002 Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. (1922). Beiträge zur sozialpsychologie des Wickham, H. (2007) Reshaping Data with the Package. Journal of haushuhns [Observation on the social psychology of domestic Statistical Software 21 (12) fowls]. Zeitschrift Für Psychologie Und Physiologie Der Wickham, H. (2011) The Split-Apply-Combine Strategy for Data Sinnesorgane, Zeitschrif. Retrieved from https://psycnet.apa.org/ Analysis. Journal of Statistical Software 40 (1) record/1926-07009-001 Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis. Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. (1935). Social behavior of birds. In A Handbook of Retrieved from https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org Social Psychology. (pp. 947–972). Worcester, MA, US: Clark Wickham, H., & Lionel, H. (2020). tidyr: tidy messy data. Retrieved from University Press. https://cran.r-project.org/package=tidyr Smirnova, A. A., Lazareva, O. F., & Zorina, Z. A. (2000). Use of number Wickham, H., François, R., Henry, L., & Kirill, M. (2020). dplyr: a by crows: investigation by matching and oddity learning. Journal of grammar of data manipulation. Retrieved from https://cran.r- the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 73(2), 163–176. https://doi. project.org/package=dplyr org/10.1901/jeab.2000.73-163 Wilson, M. L., Kahlenberg, S. M., Wells, M., & Wrangham, R. W. Vallortigara, G. (1992a). Right hemisphere advantage for social recogni- (2012). Ecological and social factors affect the occurrence and out- tion in the chick. Neuropsychologia, 30(9), 761–768. https://doi.org/ comes of intergroup encounters in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90080-6 83(1), 277–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.11.004 Vallortigara, G. (1992b). Affiliation and aggression as related to gender in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Journal of Comparative Workman, L., & Andrew, R. J. (1989). Simultaneous changes in behav- Psychology, 106(1), 53–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036. iour and in lateralization during the development of male and female 106.1.53 domestic chicks. Animal Behaviour, 38(4), 596–605. https://doi.org/ Vallortigara, G. (2015). Foundations of number and space representations 10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80004-1 in non-human species (pp. 35–66). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0- Xia, L., Emmerton, J., Siemann, M., & Delius, J. D. (2001). Pigeons 12-420133-0.00002-8 (Columba livia) learn to link numerosities with symbols. Journal Vallortigara, G. (2017). An animal’s sense of number. In J. Adams, P. of Comparative Psychology, 115(1), 83–91. https://doi.org/10. Barmby, & A. Mesoudi (Eds.), The nature and Development of 1037/0735-7036.115.1.83 Mathematics: Cross Disciplinary Perspectives on Cognition, Yamaguchi,S.,Aoki,N.,Kitajima, T., Iikubo, E., Katagiri, S., Learning and Culture (pp. 43–65). Routledge. Matsushima, T., & Homma, K. J. (2012). Thyroid hormone deter- Vallortigara, G., & Andrew, R. J. (1991). Lateralization of response by mines the start of the sensitive period of imprinting and primes later chicks to change in a model partner. Animal Behaviour, 41(2), 187– learning. Nature Communications, 3(1), 1081. https://doi.org/10. 194. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80470-1 1038/ncomms2088 Vallortigara, G., Cailotto, M., & Zanforlin, M. (1990). Sex differences in Yang, T.-I., & Chiao, C.-C. (2016). Number sense and state-dependent social reinstatement motivation of the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) valuation in cuttlefish. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: revealed by runway tests with social and nonsocial reinforcement. Biological Sciences, 283(1837), 20161379. https://doi.org/10. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104(4), 361–367. https://doi. 1098/rspb.2016.1379 org/10.1037/0735-7036.104.4.361 Vallortigara, G, & Versace, E. (2018). Filial imprinting. In Encyclopedia Publisher’snote Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic- of Animal Cognition and Behavior (pp. 1–4). Cham: Springer tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829- 6_1989-1
Learning & Behavior – Springer Journals
Published: Oct 6, 2020
You can share this free article with as many people as you like with the url below! We hope you enjoy this feature!
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.