Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
(1992)
The smoking consequences questionnaire : the sub j ective utility of smoking in college students ,
(1985)
Cognitive structures , cognitive processes and cognitivebehavior modi cation ,
M. Masson (1995)
A distributed memory model of semantic priming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21
J. Bargh, R. Bond, W. Lombardi, Mary Tota (1986)
The additive nature of chronic and temporary sources of construct accessibility.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50
S. Ames, A. Stacy (1998)
Implicit Cognition in the Prediction of Substance Use Among Drug OffendersPsychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12
C. Sadler (1990)
The risk-takers.Nursing times, 86 18
A. Stacy, M. Newcomb, P. Bentler (1991)
Cognitive motivation and drug use: a 9-year longitudinal study.Journal of abnormal psychology, 100 4
(1997)
Automatic and non-automatic processing of alcohol-associations in heavy vs. light drinkers
John Anderson (1983)
A spreading activation theory of memory.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22
C. McCusker, B. Gettings (1997)
Automaticity of cognitive biases in addictive behaviours: further evidence with gamblers.The British journal of clinical psychology, 36 ( Pt 4)
W. Miller (1985)
Motivation for treatment: a review with special emphasis on alcoholism.Psychological bulletin, 98 1
(1991)
The theory of planned behaviour, Organisational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
G. Connors, A. Tarbox, L. Faillace (1993)
Changes in alcohol expectancies and drinking behavior among treated problem drinkers.Journal of studies on alcohol, 54 6
B. Johnsen, J. Laberg, W. Cox, A. Vaksdal, K. Hugdahl (1994)
Alcoholic subjects' attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-related words.Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8
B. Jones, J. McMahon (1996)
A comparison of positive and negative alcohol expectancy and value and their multiplicative composite as predictors of post-treatment abstinence survivorship.Addiction, 91 1
N. Cooney, R. Gillespie, L. Baker, R. Kaplan (1987)
Cognitive changes after alcohol cue exposure.Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 55 2
C. McCusker, G. Roberts, J. Douthwaite, Elwyn Williams (1995)
Teenagers and illicit drug use : expanding the 'user vs non-user' dichotomyJournal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 5
S. Moscovici (1993)
The return of the unconsciousSocial Research, 60
(1989)
Drug aubsersEds) Cognitive Therapy in Clinical Practice
R. Warren (1972)
Stimulus encoding and memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94
M. Sayette, M. Hufford (1994)
Effects of cue exposure and deprivation on cognitive resources in smokers.Journal of abnormal psychology, 103 4
E. Tulving, D. Schacter (1990)
Priming and human memory systems.Science, 247 4940
B. Leigh, A. Stacy (1998)
Individual differences in memory associations involving the positive and negative outcomes of alcohol use.Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12
Hiiltine Joffe (1996)
AIDS research and prevention: a social representational approach.The British journal of medical psychology, 69 ( Pt 3)
R. Shiffrin, W. Schneider (1977)
Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory.Psychological Review, 84
(1999)
Evidence of memory biases for positive alcohol expectancies in problem drinkers, Paper presented at the Annual Conference of The
K. Leung, C. McCusker (1999)
Accessibility and availability of smoking-related associations in smokersAddiction Research, 7
D. Meyer, R. Schvaneveldt (1971)
Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations.Journal of experimental psychology, 90 2
(1994)
The effects of cue-exposure on attention in male alcoholics
A. Collins, E. Loftus (1975)
A spreading-activation theory of semantic processingPsychological Review, 82
B. Rather, M. Goldman (1994)
Drinking-related differences in the memory organization of alcohol expectancies.Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2
R. Parasuraman (1998)
The attentive brain
(1994)
Models of preventive health behaviour: comparison, critique and meta-analysis, Advances in Medical Sociology
M. Goldman (1999)
Risk for Substance Abuse: Memory as a Common Etiological PathwayPsychological Science, 10
A. Weinstein, B. Feldtkeller, A. Malizia, Sue Wilson, J. Bailey, D. Nutt (1998)
Integrating the cognitive and physiological aspects of cravingJournal of Psychopharmacology, 12
S. Grossberg (1995)
The Attentive BrainAmerican Scientist
S. Tiffany, B. Carter (1998)
Is craving the source of compulsive drug use?Journal of Psychopharmacology, 12
Ilan Yaniv, David Meyer (1987)
Activation and metacognition of inaccessible stored information: potential bases for incubation effects in problem solving.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 13 2
L. Roehrich, M. Goldman (1995)
Implicit Priming of Alcohol Expectancy Memory Processes and Subsequent Drinking BehaviorExperimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 3
(1993)
Nicotine abstinence produces contentspeci
John Schafer, Sandra Brown (1991)
Marijuana and cocaine effect expectancies and drug use patterns.Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 59 4
D. Schacter (1992)
Understanding implicit memory. A cognitive neuroscience approach.The American psychologist, 47 4
G. Logan (1988)
Toward an instance theory of automatization.Psychological Review, 95
B. Loken (1982)
Heavy smokers', light smokers', and nonsmokers' beliefs about cigarette smoking.The Journal of applied psychology, 67 5
J. Feldman, John Lynch (1988)
Self-generated validity and other effects of measurement on belief, attitude, intention, and behavior.Journal of Applied Psychology, 73
(1995)
Cognitive bias for addiction-related stimuli in smokers and problems drinkers
M. Griffiths (1994)
The role of cognitive bias and skill in fruit machine gambling.British Journal of Psychology, 85
R. Nisbett, T. Wilson (1977)
Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes.Psychological Review, 84
A. Stacy (1997)
Memory activation and expectancy as prospective predictors of alcohol and marijuana use.Journal of abnormal psychology, 106 1
S. Tiffany (1990)
A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior: role of automatic and nonautomatic processes.Psychological review, 97 2
K. Sher, M. Wood, P. Wood, G. Raskin (1996)
Alcohol outcome expectancies and alcohol use: a latent variable cross-lagged panel study.Journal of abnormal psychology, 105 4
B. Rather, M. Goldman, L. Roehrich, M. Brannick (1992)
Empirical modeling of an alcohol expectancy memory network using multidimensional scaling.Journal of abnormal psychology, 101 1
A. Stacy, S. Ames, S. Sussman, C. Dent (1996)
Implicit cognition in adolescent drug use.Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 10
J. Gray (1990)
Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition.Cognition & Emotion, 4
B. Christiansen, G. Smith, P. Roehling, M. Goldman (1989)
Using alcohol expectancies to predict adolescent drinking behavior after one year.Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 57 1
Kenneth Weingardt, A. Stacy, Barbara Leigh (1996)
Automatic activation of alcohol concepts in response to positive outcomes of alcohol use.Alcoholism, clinical and experimental research, 20 1
P. Graf, G. Mandler (1984)
Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable.Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23
M. Goldman, B. Rather (1993)
Substance Use Disorders: Cognitive Models and Architectures
An evolution in theoretical models and methodological paradigms for investigating cognitive biases in the addictions is discussed. Anomalies in traditional cognitive perspectives, and problems with the self‐report methods which underpin them, are highlighted. An emergent body of cognitive research, contextualized within the principles and paradigms of cognitive neuropsychology rather than social learning theory, is presented which, it is argued, addresses these anomalies and problems. Evidence is presented that biases in the processing of addiction‐related stimuli, and in the network of propositions which motivate addictive behaviours, occur at automatic, implicit and pre‐conscious levels of awareness. It is suggested that methods which assess such implicit cognitive biases (e.g. Stroop, memory, priming and reaction‐time paradigms) yield findings which have better predictive utility for ongoing behaviour than those biases determined by self‐report methods of introspection. The potential utility of these findings for understanding "loss of control" phenomena, and the desynchrony between reported beliefs and intentions and ongoing addictive behaviours, is discussed. Applications to the practice of cognitive therapy are considered.
Addiction – Wiley
Published: Jan 1, 2001
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.